- ...States
-
Preparation of this article was supported in part by
Andersen Consulting through Northwestern University's
Institute for the Learning Sciences.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
- ...work.
-
In some cases we wished to incorporate affective phenomena not specifically
addressed in these works (e.g., ``valence bias'') and have therefore created
computable formalisms for these phenomena based on relevant psychological
literature. We do not claim that our formalisms for these phenomena reflect
actual cognitive processes at a low level.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
- ...calculate.
-
For example, for a person to be surprised when a jet airplane crashes into
their building they must know that this is something that does not normally
occur. By contrast, a person is not surprised when the phone rings in their
office, because this is something that does normally occur. This is a true
default reasoning problem because though one event is surprising and the
other is not, the person need not have active expectations regarding
planes or telephones (see also [Ortony and Partridge1987]). We have included these
difficult-to-calculate variables because they are important theoretically,
and because in some simulation paradigms we have a pragmatic way of making
such values available to the affective reasoning machinery.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
- ...continuum.
-
The continuum for this variable, and the next three, must be measured along
the correct dimension. This is not always a straightforward problem. For
example, in the Biblical story of the two harlots that are both claiming to
be the mother of a newborn (Kings 1:3), Solomon threatens to divide the child
in half and give a piece to each of the women who claim him. The real mother
desires first that she get full possession of the child, second that she
have possession of no part of him, and least of all that she have half of
him.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
- ...was.
-
A more straightforward example would be, ``how fresh the flowers were.''
This example was chosen because it illustrates the concept that the
judgment of appealingness, and so forth, is internal to the agent, while
the degree of that appealingness may be set by an external,
albeit difficult to measure, value.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
- ...1.
-
It is not clear what the relationship between this variable and the
friendship-animosity variable should be. We have created this as a
distinct variable to handle such instances as animosities that arise
in the course of family life, and so forth. To wit: if a family member
cheats you of your inheritance it may be a much more intense experience
than if ``the government'' does, regardless of whether the relationship
to that family member is one of friendship or animosity.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
- ...anxiety.
- We treat depression and anxiety as mood-biasing
phenomena rather than as moods themselves, in keeping with much
cognitive psychology research (e.g., [Beck1967]).
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
- ...needed.
-
Even the notion of emotional intensity itself is underspecified since a
full treatment would have to account for such parameters as the amplitudes
of multiple intensity peaks, their durations, and so forth. (c.f.,
[Frijda et al.
1992]).
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.